Case Control Study Open Access
Copyright ©The Author(s) 2016. Published by Baishideng Publishing Group Inc. All rights reserved.
World J Psychiatr. Sep 22, 2016; 6(3): 311-321
Published online Sep 22, 2016. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v6.i3.311
Voxel-based magnetic resonance imaging investigation of poor and preserved clinical insight in people with schizophrenia
Adegboyega Sapara, Michael A Cooke, Veena Kumari, Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
Dominic H Ffytche, Department of Old Age Psychiatry and Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
Steven CR Williams, Department of Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
Veena Kumari, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
Author contributions: Sapara A, Ffytche DH and Kumari V designed the study; Cooke MA carried out the neuropsychological assessments; Williams SCR assisted with neuroimaging data acquisition; Sapara A performed all analyses and wrote the manuscript under Ffytche DH and Kumari V’s joint supervision.
Supported by The Wellcome Trust, United Kingdom and was carried out as part of the first author’s PhD research under Professor Veena Kumari and Dr Dominic ffytche’s supervision, Nos. 067427 and 072298; Professor Kumari is part funded by the Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health at the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience King’s College London, and the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, United Kingdom (to Kumari V).
Institutional review board statement: The study procedures had the approval (reference number 209/02) of the ethics committee of the Institute of Psychiatry and South London and Maudsley Foundation NHS Trust, London.
Informed consent statement: All participants provided written informed consent.
Conflict-of-interest statement: None of the authors declare any conflict of interest in this study.
Data sharing statement: The anonymised dataset is available from the first author and the corresponding author.
Open-Access: This article is an open-access article which was selected by an in-house editor and fully peer-reviewed by external reviewers. It is distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/
Correspondence to: Veena Kumari, PhD, Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom. veena.kumari@kcl.ac.uk
Telephone: +44-207-8480233
Received: March 2, 2016
Peer-review started: March 2, 2016
First decision: April 15, 2016
Revised: June 23, 2016
Accepted: July 11, 2016
Article in press: July 13, 2016
Published online: September 22, 2016
Processing time: 90 Days and 12.2 Hours

Abstract
AIM

To define regional grey-matter abnormalities in schizophrenia patients with poor insight (Insight-), relative to patients with preserved clinical insight (Insight+), and healthy controls.

METHODS

Forty stable schizophrenia outpatients (20 Insight- and 20 Insight+) and 20 healthy controls underwent whole brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Insight in all patients was assessed using the Birchwood Insight Scale (BIS; a self-report measure). The two patient groups were pre-selected to match on most clinical and demographic parameters but, by design, they had markedly distinct BIS scores. Voxel-based morphometry employed in SPM8 was used to examine group differences in grey matter volumes across the whole brain.

RESULTS

The three participant groups were comparable in age [F(2,57) = 0.34, P = 0.71] and the patient groups did not differ in age at illness onset [t(38) = 0.87, P = 0.39]. Insight- and Insight+ patient groups also did not differ in symptoms on the Positive and Negative Syndromes scale (PANSS): Positive symptoms [t(38) = 0.58, P = 0.57], negative symptoms [t(38) = 0.61, P = 0.55], general psychopathology [t(38) = 1.30, P = 0.20] and total PANSS scores [t(38) = 0.21, P = 0.84]. The two patient groups, as expected, varied significantly in the level of BIS-assessed insight [t(38) = 12.11, P < 0.001]. MRI results revealed lower fronto-temporal, parahippocampal, occipital and cerebellar grey matter volumes in Insight- patients, relative to Insight+ patients and healthy controls (for all clusters, family-wise error corrected P < 0.05). Insight+ patient and healthy controls did not differ significantly (P > 0.20) from each other.

CONCLUSION

Our findings demonstrate a clear association between poor clinical insight and smaller fronto-temporal, occipital and cerebellar grey matter volumes in stable long-term schizophrenia patients.

Key Words: Psychosis; Insight; Grey matter volumes; Fronto-temporal; Neural networks; Birchwood insight scale

Core tip: Poor clinical insight is the most prevalent symptom in patients with schizophrenia and is of growing importance due to its direct association with poor clinical outcomes, such as frequent relapses and hospital admissions. This study identified significantly reduced fronto-temporal, parahippocampal, occipital and cerebellar grey matter volumes in Insight- patients relative to both Insight+ patients and healthy controls. The involvement of multiple brain areas and corresponding neural networks supports the theory that clinical insight, as a neurological function, is not confined to specific neuroanatomical regions but probably a function of a complex neurocognitive interplay with contributions from multiple neural networks.



INTRODUCTION

Nearly a century ago, Kraepelin (1919) observed that schizophrenia patients often had “no real understanding of the gravity of their disorder” and regularly disputed that they suffer from a mental illness[1]. In the 1930s, Lewis described clinical insight as having “a correct attitude to a morbid change in one’s self”[2,3] and low clinical insight is the most prevalent symptom occurring in about 97% of schizophrenia patients[2,4]. Impaired insight in schizophrenia is of growing importance due to its direct correlation with poor clinical outcomes, such as frequent relapses and hospital admissions[5], poor compliance with medication and treatment plans[6-8], severe psychopathology[9], greater suicidal tendencies and self-injurious behaviour[9-12]. Some studies reporting positive correlations between improvement in clinical insight and better global clinical impression and clinical outcome scores[13] have further suggested the adoption of clinical insight as a possible therapeutic target in schizophrenia patients[14].

Similarities between impaired insight in schizophrenia and unawareness of neurological deficits such as anosognosia, first described in patients with acute brain lesions with left-sided hemiplegia who were unaware of the impairments in their paralysed limbs[15,16], led to the notion that both phenomena share a common neurological basis[17-19] and prompted investigations of neuroanatomical abnormalities in relation to impaired clinical insight in schizophrenia. Earliest studies, using computerized tomography (CT) scan, reported significant and direct associations between impaired clinical insight and ventricular enlargement[20], total insight scores and total brain volumes[21] and a linear relationship between global cortical atrophy and impaired clinical insight[22]. These studies all concluded that there is a significant association between whole brain volume loss and impaired clinical insight in schizophrenia. Structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies also reported correlations between impaired clinical insight and smaller regional grey matter volumes, including the frontal lobe, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), posterior cingulate, temporal and parietal lobes[23-28]. More recently, correlations have been reported between impaired insight and smaller right posterior insula volumes[29], smaller grey matter volumes of the right ventro-lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC)[30], left ventrolateral PFC, right dorsolateral PFC, insula, bilateral premotor area and the putamen; and reduced white matter volumes of the right superior longitudinal fasciculum, left corona radiata, left forceps minor and bilateral cingulum[31].

Although most studies have reported a correlation between brain volume loss and impaired insight, some studies failed to find any correlation between clinical insight and either ventricular or total/regional brain volumes[3,32,33], while others reported associations between impaired clinical insight and increased (rather than decreased) right medial orbitofrontal cortex grey matter volumes[28], and between symptom misattribution and increased grey matter volumes in bilateral caudate regions, right thalamus, left insula, putamen and cerebellum[34]. Bassitt et al[35] found no significant inverse correlation between total or regional grey matter volumes and clinical insight but, contrary to their expectations, observed a positive correlation between degree of insight impairment and the left medial PFC and ACC grey matter volumes, which they attributed to higher doses of antipsychotics given to patients with impaired clinical insight in their sample. The marked variation in findings may be due to the use of different brain volumetric assessment techniques, the heterogeneity of clinical insight measures and varying clinical characteristics of schizophrenia patients studied[25,35,36].

The aim of the present study was to characterise grey matter alterations in stable long-term schizophrenia outpatients with impaired clinical insight by directly comparing them, for the first time to our knowledge, with schizophrenia outpatients with preserved clinical insight, matched on average for age, sex and relevant demographic and clinical characteristics. Our approach of utilising the two extremes of the insight distribution should yield the largest structural difference in relation to insight. We also compared how these distinct groups of patients might differ from healthy controls, matched on average on age and sex of the patient groups. Based on the findings (where positive) of existing studies involving solely or predominantly chronic patient samples, we hypothesised that, patients with impaired insight (Insight-) will show smaller frontal and temporal regional grey matter volumes compared to patients with preserved insight (Insight+) and healthy controls. This hypothesis also has support from previous studies showing, on average, poor cognitive function in patients with impaired insight[25,37,38] and a positive association between grey matter volumes of these regions and a range of cognitive functions in schizophrenia[39].

MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants and study design

This study included 60 right-handed participants. Forty of these were patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, confirmed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID)[40]. The patients formed two groups of 20 patients each, pre-selected to have preserved and impaired insight, out of a larger pool of 70 stable community patients. The assessment of insight and differentiating criteria are described in detail under “clinical assessment”. All included patients were required to be: (1) on well established antipsychotic medication doses for ≥ 3 mo; (2) in the stable (chronic) phase of the illness; and (3) ≥ 2 years from illness onset. Twenty healthy controls screened to exclude neuropsychiatric conditions and matched, on average, for age and sex of the patients were studied for comparison purposes. Ethics approval was granted by the ethics committee of the Institute of Psychiatry and South London and Maudsley Foundation NHS Trust, London. All participants provided written informed consent.

Clinical assessment

Birchwood Insight Scale (BIS)[41], a self-rated questionnaire, was used to assess insight in all patients. The BIS measures three different aspects of clinical insight[2]: (1) the awareness of the presence of a mental disorder (2nd and 7th statement); (2) the awareness of the need for treatment (3rd, 6th statement); and (3) the ability to label symptoms as abnormal (1st and 8th statement). Each individual BIS statement (8 in total) is rated and given a score of one for unsure, and either 0 or 2 for agree and disagree, depending on whether agreeing with the statement depicts preserved clinical insight (all statements are corrected for response valence). As we did not include any inpatients, Item 4 “My stay in hospital is necessary” was deleted, thus yielding a maximum possible score of 14, compared with a maximum possible score of 16 in the full scale BIS. In operationalising the BIS, Birchwood et al[41] classified preserved insight as having a minimum score of 9 (out of 14). In this study, we defined “preserved insight” as a minimum score of 13 (out of 14) and “impaired insight” as a score of 8 or below. This rather conservative method was designed to ensure that the two groups had distinct levels of insight and also to eliminate those with partial clinical insight levels. All patients were supervised during the completion of the BIS. The BIS has acceptable internal consistency (α = 0.75) and one week test-retest reliability (r = 0.90 for the total score[41]), and insight assessed on the BIS correlates positively with scores on other measures of insight[10,26,42]. For sample characterization purposes, symptoms in patients were assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scales (PANSS[43]). In addition, predicted IQ of all study participants was measured using the National Adult Reading Test (NART[44]).

Image acquisition and processing

Whole brain MRI scans were acquired for all study participants using a 1.5 Tesla GE NV/I Signa system (General Electric, Milwaukee WI, United States) at the Maudsley Hospital, London. A series of sagittal fast gradient echo scout images were obtained to correct for head tilt and to orient subsequent images relative to the anterior-commissure/posterior-commissure line and the interhemispheric fissure. A 3-D inversion recovery prepared fast spoiled GRASS sequence was applied to acquire T1-weighted images in the axial plane with 1.5 mm contiguous sections (TR = 18 ms, T1 = 450 ms, TE = 5.1 ms, flip angle = 20o with one data average and a 256 × 256 × 128 voxel matrix). Acquisition parameters were selected employing a sophisticated image simulation[45]. All MRI images were converted into ANALYZE format (ANALYZE software, BRU, Mayo Foundation, Rochester, MN) and pre-processed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8; http://wwwfil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) running in MATLAB 2006a (MathWorks, Natick, MA). Customised T1-weighted templates of the whole brain, grey matter, white matter and cerebro-spinal fluid (CSF) were created for patient and healthy participant groups separately, and also for the whole study sample combined.

Statistical analysis

Demographic and clinical measures: Possible group differences in age, education and NART IQ were examined using analyses of variance (ANOVAs), and significant Group effects were followed by independent sample t-tests. Possible differences between the two patient groups in clinical variables (age at illness onset, PANSS symptom scores and medication) were examined using independent sample t-tests. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 22, with alpha level for significance testing maintained at P≤ 0.05 (two-tailed), unless stated otherwise.

MRI: Group differences (healthy controls vs Insight- patients, health controls vs Insight+ paitents, and Insight+vs Insight- patients) in grey matter volumes, across the whole brain, were examined using ANOVA in SPM8 (height threshold P < 0.005; familywise-error (FWE)-corrected at the cluster level P < 0.05). To rule out the possibility that any observed group differences were due to trend-level Group differences in education and IQ (see RESULTS, demographic and clinical measures), group differences in grey matter volumes were re-evaluated using analysis of co-variance, with education and IQ entered as co-variates.

RESULTS
Demographic and clinical characteristics

The three participant groups did not differ in age [F(2,57 = 0.34, P = 0.71]. There were trend level effects of Group in years of education [F(2,57) = 2.60, P = 0.08] and NART IQ [F(2,57) = 2.67, P = 0.08]. Healthy controls spent more years in education than Insight- patients [t(38) = 2.11, P = 0.04] but differed only at a trend level when compared with Insight+ patients [t(38) = 1.77, P = 0.08]. Healthy controls also had higher NART IQ than Insight- patients [t(38) = 2.47, P = 0.02] but did not differ from Insight+ patients [t(38) = 1.19, P = 0.24]. There were no significant differences the Insight- and Insight+ patient groups in education [t(38) = 0.06, P = 0.95] and NART IQ [t(38) = 1.04, P = 0.31] (Table 1). The two patient groups were similar in age at illness onset [t(38) = 0.87, P = 0.39], positive symptoms [t(38) = 0.58, P = 0.57], negative symptoms [t(38) = 0.61, P = 0.55], general psychopathology [t(38) = 1.30, P = 0.20] and total PANSS symptoms [t(38) = 0.21, P = 0.84]. Patients in the two groups were on a range of typical and atypical antipsychotics (Table 1) but received, on average, similar doses of antipsychotic medication [t(38) = 0.86, P = 0.40]. The Insight+ patient group, confirming our insight-based pre-selection, had significantly higher BIS score than the Insight- group [t(38) = 12.11, P < 0.001].

Table 1 Demographics and clinical characteristics of the study groups.
Healthy controls (n = 20; 15 male, 5 female)
Patients insight+ group (n = 20; 16 male, 4 female)
Patients insight- group (n = 20; 16 male, 4 female)
Mean (SD)RangeMean (SD)RangeMean (SD)Range
Demographics
Age (yr)35.25 (10.93)20-5936.15 (10.54)19-5437.80 (7.85)22-49
Education (yr)15.05 (2.86)10-2013.45 (2.86)9-2013.40 (2.01)11-19
Predicted IQ (NART)113.10 (9.91)91-128109.20 (10.80)86-122106.10 (7.87)90-119
Clinical characteristics
BIS11.65 (0.57)13-145.88 (2.05)1-8
Age at illness onset (yr)25.90 (8.72)13-4823.85 (5.84)10-37
PANSS positive symptoms16.15 (5.38)8-2517.05 (4.43)8-23
PANSS negative symptoms17.20 (4.38)7-2718.15 (5.46)8-27
PANSS general psychopathology34.35 (7.36)24-5631.55 (6.27)21-40
PANSS total symptoms67.70 (14.90)43-10866.75 (14.02)37-86
Medication (chlorpromazine equivalent in mg)461.21 (333.95)100-1600556.63 (366.49)200-1367
Medication type
Atypical antipsychotics18 (9 olanzapine, 5 risperidone, 3 clozapine, 1 quetiapine)13 (7 olanzapine, 3 clozapine, 1 aripiprazole, 1 amisulpride, 1 risperidone)
Typical antipsychotics2 (1 sulpiride, 1 haloperidol)5 (2 flupenthixol, 1 fluphenazine, 1 sulpiride, 1 haloperidol)
Both--2 (1 on clozapine + levomepromazine, 1 zuclopenthixol + aripiprazole)
MRI: Group effects in regional grey matter volumes

Group differences in brain MRI grey matter volumes are presented in Table 2, and described below.

Table 2 Group differences in grey matter volumes (height threshold P < 0.005).
GroupsBASizeSideMNI
T valueCluster P FWE-corrected unless in italicsVoxel P FWE-corrected
XYZ
Insight+ > Insight- patients
Superior temporal gyrus2246555R63-354.910.0010.020
4520-334.740.034
66-844.680.040
Precentral gyrus466-5224.550.057
Inferior frontal gyrus47541904.520.063
Precentral gyrus6640264.400.088
Postcentral gyrus4366-8164.330.106
parahippocampus28140-274.070.406
Inferior frontal gyrus47103898L-4115-64.81< 0.0010.027
Middle frontal gyrus9-3719354.740.034
Inferior frontal gyrus47-3715-104.730.035
-3520-104.540.059
Precentral gyrus44-59874.390.091
Superior temporal gyrus22-62-484.360.097
Precentral gyrus6-60464.330.107
Middle temporal gyrus21-35-3-234.270.126
parahippocampal gyrus20-34-5-284.160.166
Cuneus1835993L-5-8354.430.0030.082
Cerebellum-R35-90-174.260.129
Cuneus18R26-93-183.900.305
Cerebellum-R4-6123.880.317
Cuneus18R5-98103.500.630
R5-9633.440.674
Cerebellum-L-36-82-153.380.730
Insight- > Insight+ patients
Nil significant
Healthy controls > Insight- patients
Inferior frontal gyrus4735300L-4919-34.630.0040.046
Superior temporal gyrus22-60134.300.115
Inferior frontal gyrus47-4118-54.210.144
-3822-84.190.153
-36-1-143.860.333
Inferior temporal gyrus20-28-14-413.610.530
Parahippocamal gyrus34-134-233.580.552
Middle frontal gyrus11-4240-193.390.722
Inferior occipital gyrus1811168L-38-92-24.510.0340.065
Middle occipital gyrus19-52-76-104.290.117
-48-80-143.960.266
-49-8173.900.302
Middle temporal gyrus39/-53-72223.370.740
19-52-74183.330.768
-49-76203.290.797
Cerebellum-25235R35-90-174.460.0160.074
(posterior lobe)11-90-374.210.146
Occipital lobe1823-94-184.010.238
Cerebellum34-85-403.930.355
(posterior lobe)38-82-413.910.489
Insight- patients > healthy controls
Nil significant
Healthy control > Insight+ patients
Nil significant
Insight+ patients > healthy controls
Nil significant

Insight-vs Insight+ patients: Compared to Insight- patients, Insight+ patients had larger grey matter volumes in the inferior frontal and precentral gyri, superior and middle temporal gyri, parahippocampus, cuneus and cerebellum of both cerebral hemispheres (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Figure 1 Images showing regions of decreased grey matter volume in the impaired insight patient group, relative to the preserved insight patient and healthy controls (maps thresholded at P = 0. 005; left = right).

Healthy controls vs Insight- patients: Compared to Insight- patients, healthy controls had larger grey matter volumes in the left inferior and middle frontal gyri, left superior, middle and inferior temporal gyri, left parahippocampus, right cerebellum, and bilateral superior, middle and inferior occipital gyri (Figure 1).

Healthy controls vs Insight+ patients: There were no significant differences between healthy controls and Insight+ patients.

Group differences after co-varying for education and predicted IQ

Differences in grey matter volumes (noted earlier) between healthy controls and Insight- patients remained present but with reduced significance when we co-varied for education and IQ (Table 3). Group differences between Insight- and Insight+ patients, however, were not affected.

Table 3 Group differences in grey matter volumes after co-varying for education and National Adult Reading Test IQ (height threshold P < 0.005).
GroupsBASizeSideMNI
T valueCluster P FWE-corrected unless shown in italicsVoxel P FWE-corrected
XYZ
Insight+ > Insight- patients
Superior Temporal gyrus2237261R63-354.700.0020.044
4520-334.560.066
66-844.450.088
Precentral gyrus466-5224.440.092
Inferior frontal gyrus47541904.390.103
Precentral gyrus6640264.280.137
Postcentral gyrus4366-8164.150.192
Inferior frontal gyrus4765047L-4216-44.65< 0.0010.050
-3814-84.650.052
-3618-104.520.073
Middle frontal gyrus9-3719354.520.073
Superior temporal gyrus22-61-274.280.139
Precentral gyrus44-59994.170.184
Parahippocampual gyrus21-34-3-364.100.213
Cuneus1824291L-5-8354.320.0140.125
Cerebellum-R35-90-174.170.181
Cuneus18R26-93-183.730.466
Cerebellum-R4-6123.800.409
Cuneus18R5-98103.350.787
Medial frontal gyrus1016854L06033.980.0500.285
Superior frontal gyrus9051263.640.544
Insight- > Insight+ patients
Nil significant
Healthy controls > Insight- patients
Inferior frontal gyrus479770L-5119-23.680.0360.511
Superior temporal gyrus38-215-243.350.786
Inferior frontal gyrus47-2618-73.340.796
Parahippocampal gyrus34-164-233.290.827
Inferior occipital gyrus184935L-38-92-23.920.1220.323
Middle occipital gyrus19-52-76-103.700.494
-44-8383.370.775
Middle temporal gyrus18-43-81133.220.873
Cerebellum (posterior lobe)-6085R35-90-173.680.0890.304
11-90-373.600.378
Occipital lobe1828-94-163.320.656
23-94-183.260.713
Insight- patients > healthy controls
Nil significant
Healthy controls > Insight+ patients
Nil significant
Insight+ patients > healthy controls
Nil significant
DISCUSSION

In this study, we directly compared two matched groups of schizophrenia patients but with distinct levels of clinical insight (Insight-vs Insight+) and investigated how they differ from each other and also from healthy controls in regional grey matter volumes examined using voxel-based morphometry (VBM) technique. We tested the hypothesis that Insight- patients will show smaller frontal and temporal grey matter volumes compared to Insight+ patients. All three participant groups were comparable in age and the two patient groups were similar in all demographic and clinical parameters, including age at illness onset, years of education, NART IQ, symptoms (PANSS scores) and doses of medication prescribed. Insight- patients, however, had lower IQ and fewer years in education than healthy controls. Although, on average, lower IQ as well as deficits in many specific cognitive domains in patients with schizophrenia, relative to the healthy population, are commonly reported[46], our study suggests that this may be particularly true for those with impaired insight[37] and in turn may also explain the finding of significantly fewer years in education in the Insight- (but not Insight+) patient group, compared with the healthy controls. The patient groups scored at opposing ends of the BIS scale; this allows for the interpretation of observed neuroanatomical differences in relation to clinical insight levels of the respective patient group.

As hypothesized, we found that Insight- patients had smaller grey matter volumes than Insight+ patients, bilaterally in the frontal and temporal lobes (mainly in the inferior frontal and precentral gyri and superior and middle temporal gyri), as well as in the parahippocamal gyrus, occipital lobes (including the cuneus) and the cerebellum. Insight- patients also showed similar grey matter deficits, particularly on the left, when compared to healthy controls (Figure 1).

Our findings of smaller fronto-temporal regional grey matter volumes are in accordance with previous imaging studies, that used the “Region of Interest” (ROI) approach and found a significant and direct correlation between smaller frontal areas, including the dorsolateral PFC, inferior frontal and middle frontal gyri[22,26-28,47,48] and impaired clinical insight. Early reports of poor executive functioning in schizophrenia patients with impaired insight, similar to those with frontal lobe lesions, initiated the interest in the integrity of the frontal lobe in schizophrenia. Since then, several studies[26,30,31,47], including this one, have reported frontal neuroanatomical abnormalities in relation to impaired clinical insight in schizophrenia. Some functional imaging studies have further associated aberrant frontal functional MRI activity with impaired clinical insight during working memory[49], self-reflection[50], self-monitoring[51] and self-awareness tasks[52] in schizophrenia. In addition, earlier correlational VBM studies have also reported associations between smaller superior and middle temporal lobe grey matter volumes and impaired clinical insight[23,48].

Our other finding of smaller cuneus and occipital grey matter volumes in Insight- patients is also broadly in agreement with the earlier reported association between poor symptom relabelling dimension of clinical insight and smaller grey matter volumes of the precuneus, cuneus and medial occipital gyrus by Morgan et al[25]. Unlike Morgan et al[25], we did not investigate preferential or predominant contribution of particular insight dimensions because the BIS subscale scores in our sample were highly positively correlated with each other (rho = 0.50-0.882; P < 0.001). This might be due to our sampling methods that ensured that our Insight- and Insight+ patient groups had markedly different insight levels, possibly in all domains. Other VBM studies have also reported an association between the smaller precuneus grey matter volumes and lower insight in schizophrenia[23]. The role of the precuneus has been described in the facilitation of increased awareness into one’s mental states[23,53] and has also been implicated, in conjunction with other midline structures, in the self-appraisal processes[54,55]. Compared to anterior cortical regions, much less is known about the involvement of posterior medial cortices due to the dearth of research into the contributions of these brain regions to various aspects of psychotic disorders[25]. In our recent study, we found further evidence of functional contributions from the precuneus, as well as the cerebellum, in supporting neural activities sub-serving the preservation of insight in schizophrenia patients[49].

There have been previous reports of cerebellar atrophy, on average, in schizophrenia patients[56]. A previous study[48] also observed a significant association between impaired clinical insight and reduced bilateral cerebellar grey matter volumes in schizophrenia, and that this relationship was not associated with any specific dimension of clinical insight. Other studies have described the involvement of the cerebellum in higher cognitive functioning, with its extensive connectivity with limbic structures, including the parahippocampal gyrus, and associated cortical areas involved in cognition and executive function[57,58], and this has been implicated in the neuropathology of schizophrenia and poor clinical insight[48,59]. Our recent finding of increased cerebellar activity, detected using fMRI, in Insight+ patients compared to Insight- patients, during a working memory task, also indicated cerebellar involvement in the preservation of clinical insight in schizophrenia[49].

In accordance with the observations made by other studies, we also found grey matter reductions in many areas in Insight- patients, compared to healthy controls[48]. These differences remained, but became less significant, after we co-varied for education and NART IQ. Co-varying for education and NART IQ had no effects on grey matter volume differences between preserved and Insight- patient groups, most likely because these two groups were comparable on these parameters.

Strengths and limitations

We employed a direct comparison method between distinct groups of schizophrenia patients (Insight- and Insight+) with closely matched demographic and clinical qualities, thereby facilitating valid comparisons and inferences. The study also had 60 participants (n = 20 per group) and thus was adequately powered for the observations made. We were, however, limited in our ability to explore the effects of sex on brain volumes and in the observed group differences, as our sample was predominantly male. Nonetheless, male:female ratios were similar and any possible effect is expected to be uniform in all groups. Also, although the patient groups were comparable in all relevant areas, our healthy controls had more education than our patient groups, and had higher IQ scores than Insight- patient group, although co-varying for these differences did not change the pattern of observed group differences. By adopting a direct comparison method between matched patient groups at the extremes of insight measures, we minimised confounding effects of partial insight levels and were able to exclude overall effects of schizophrenia on brain volumes. However, in as much as we endeavoured that our two patient groups are highly comparable but for their insight levels, there are possibilities of other differential properties, such as brain functional properties, which could possibly contribute to our findings. Lastly, patients in both the Insight+ and Insight- groups were on a range of atypical and typical antipsychotics (Table 1) which vary in their pharmacological profiles[60,61] as well as in their effects on brain volumes[62]. This may have influenced the results we observed in this study.

In conclusion, schizophrenia patients with impaired insight patients have smaller fronto-temporal, parahippocampal, occipital and cerebellar grey matter volumes, compared with preserved insight schizophrenia patients and healthy controls. The involvement of multiple brain areas and corresponding neural networks supports the theory that clinical insight, as a neurological function, is not confined to specific neuroanatomical regions in the brain but probably a function of a complex neurocognitive interplay with contributions from neural networks, including working memory and executive functioning, self-monitoring and awareness and others[19,23,49,63,64].

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We are grateful to the MRI unit, Maudsley Hospital for their help with data acquisition.

COMMENTS
Background

Impaired insight in schizophrenia is found to have a direct correlation with poor clinical outcomes, such as frequent relapses and hospital admissions, poor compliance with medication, greater suicidal tendencies and self-injurious behaviour. Some studies reporting positive correlations between improvement in clinical insight and better clinical outcomes have further suggested the adoption of clinical insight as a possible therapeutic target in schizophrenia patients.

Research frontiers

The ability to target insight therapeutically is highly complex and remains elusive to most methods trialled so far. The identification of the underpinning neural correlates of clinical insight will aid the development of specific treatment strategies aimed at improving insight in schizophrenia.

Innovations and breakthroughs

The study reported in this manuscript is distinct from all previous studies in this area (mostly correlational) in that it identifies regional grey matter abnormalities in stable schizophrenia outpatients with impaired clinical insight, relative to those with preserved clinical insight (impaired and preserved insight groups scoring at extreme ends of a multidimensional insight scale but matched on age, sex and other symptoms) as well healthy controls, using a categorical approach. The authors found a clear association between impaired clinical insight and smaller fronto-temporal, occipital and cerebellar grey matter volumes in stable long-term schizophrenia patients.

Applications

Clinical insight, as a neurological function, is likely to be dependent on complex neurocognitive interplay with contributions from multiple neural networks.

Terminology

Voxel-based-morphometry is a neuroimaging analysis technique in which structural brain properties are examined on a voxel-by-voxel basis and reported in standardized coordinates. Clinical insight refers to a patient’s complex state of awareness of his or her own mental disorder.

Peer-review

The study is well designed and the manuscript is clearly written and easy to read all throughout.

Footnotes

Manuscript source: Invited manuscript

Specialty type: Psychiatry

Country of origin: United Kingdom

Peer-review report classification

Grade A (Excellent): A

Grade B (Very good): B

Grade C (Good): C

Grade D (Fair): 0

Grade E (Poor): 0

P- Reviewer: Belli H, Eduardo JA, Tampi RR S- Editor: Ji FF L- Editor: A E- Editor: Lu YJ

References
1.  Amador XF, Seckinger RA. The assessment of insight: A methodological review. Psychiatr Ann. 1997;27:798-805.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 15]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 15]  [Article Influence: 0.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
2.  David AS. Insight and psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;156:798-808.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 755]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 695]  [Article Influence: 20.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
3.  Ouzir M, Azorin JM, Adida M, Boussaoud D, Battas O. Insight in schizophrenia: from conceptualization to neuroscience. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2012;66:167-179.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 20]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 23]  [Article Influence: 1.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
4.  Sartorius N, Shapiro R, Kimura M, Barrett K. WHO international pilot study of schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1972;2:422-425.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 72]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 73]  [Article Influence: 1.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
5.  Kelly BD, Clarke M, Browne S, McTigue O, Kamali M, Gervin M, Kinsella A, Lane A, Larkin C, O’Callaghan E. Clinical predictors of admission status in first episode schizophrenia. Eur Psychiatry. 2004;19:67-71.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 62]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 62]  [Article Influence: 3.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
6.  McEvoy JP, Freter S, Everett G, Geller JL, Appelbaum P, Apperson LJ, Roth L. Insight and the clinical outcome of schizophrenic patients. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1989;177:48-51.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
7.  David AS, Amador XF, Morgan KD.  Neuropsychological studies of insight in psychosis, in Insight and Psychosis: Awareness of Illness in Schizophrenia and Related Disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press 2004; 177-193.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
8.  Lysaker PH, Vohs J, Hillis JD, Kukla M, Popolo R, Salvatore G, Dimaggio G. Poor insight into schizophrenia: contributing factors, consequences and emerging treatment approaches. Expert Rev Neurother. 2013;13:785-793.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 77]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 83]  [Article Influence: 7.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
9.  Mintz AR, Addington J, Addington D. Insight in early psychosis: a 1-year follow-up. Schizophr Res. 2004;67:213-217.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 83]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 82]  [Article Influence: 4.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
10.  Amador XF, Strauss DH, Yale SA, Flaum MM, Endicott J, Gorman JM. Assessment of insight in psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:873-879.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 640]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 617]  [Article Influence: 19.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
11.  López-Moríñigo JD, Ramos-Ríos R, David AS, Dutta R. Insight in schizophrenia and risk of suicide: a systematic update. Compr Psychiatry. 2012;53:313-322.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 53]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 54]  [Article Influence: 4.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
12.  Sharaf AY, Ossman LH, Lachine OA. A cross-sectional study of the relationships between illness insight, internalized stigma, and suicide risk in individuals with schizophrenia. Int J Nurs Stud. 2012;49:1512-1520.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 47]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 51]  [Article Influence: 4.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
13.  Gharabawi GM, Lasser RA, Bossie CA, Zhu Y, Amador X. Insight and its relationship to clinical outcomes in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder receiving long-acting risperidone. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2006;21:233-240.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 55]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 58]  [Article Influence: 3.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
14.  McGorry PD, McConville SB. Insight in psychosis: an elusive target. Compr Psychiatry. 1999;40:131-142.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 62]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 59]  [Article Influence: 2.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
15.  Bisiach E, Vallar G, Perani D, Papagno C, Berti A. Unawareness of disease following lesions of the right hemisphere: anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianopia. Neuropsychologia. 1986;24:471-482.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 394]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 336]  [Article Influence: 8.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
16.  Pia L, Neppi-Modona M, Ricci R, Berti A. The anatomy of anosognosia for hemiplegia: a meta-analysis. Cortex. 2004;40:367-377.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 138]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 128]  [Article Influence: 6.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
17.  Amador XF, Strauss DH, Yale SA, Gorman JM. Awareness of illness in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1991;17:113-132.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 370]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 342]  [Article Influence: 10.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
18.  McGlynn SM, Schacter DL. The neuropsychology of insight: Impaired awareness of deficits in a psychiatric context. Psychiatr Ann. 1997;27:806-811.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 14]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 14]  [Article Influence: 0.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
19.  Shad MU, Keshavan MS, Tamminga CA, Cullum CM, David A. Neurobiological underpinnings of insight deficits in schizophrenia. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2007;19:437-446.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 59]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 59]  [Article Influence: 3.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
20.  Takai A, Uematsu M, Ueki H, Sone K. Insight and its related factors in chronic schizophrenic patients: A preliminary study. Eur J Psychiatry. 1992;6:159-170.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
21.  Flashman LA, McAllister TW, Andreasen NC, Saykin AJ. Smaller brain size associated with unawareness of illness in patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1167-1169.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 52]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 52]  [Article Influence: 2.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
22.  Larøi F, Fannemel M, Rønneberg U, Flekkøy K, Opjordsmoen S, Dullerud R, Haakonsen M. Unawareness of illness in chronic schizophrenia and its relationship to structural brain measures and neuropsychological tests. Psychiatry Res. 2000;100:49-58.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 102]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 96]  [Article Influence: 4.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
23.  Cooke MA, Fannon D, Kuipers E, Peters E, Williams SC, Kumari V. Neurological basis of poor insight in psychosis: a voxel-based MRI study. Schizophr Res. 2008;103:40-51.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 73]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 72]  [Article Influence: 4.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
24.  Ha TH, Youn T, Ha KS, Rho KS, Lee JM, Kim IY, Kim SI, Kwon JS. Gray matter abnormalities in paranoid schizophrenia and their clinical correlations. Psychiatry Res. 2004;132:251-260.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 92]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 95]  [Article Influence: 4.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
25.  Morgan KD, Dazzan P, Morgan C, Lappin J, Hutchinson G, Suckling J, Fearon P, Jones PB, Leff J, Murray RM. Insight, grey matter and cognitive function in first-onset psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 2010;197:141-148.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 79]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 80]  [Article Influence: 5.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
26.  Sapara A, Cooke M, Fannon D, Francis A, Buchanan RW, Anilkumar AP, Barkataki I, Aasen I, Kuipers E, Kumari V. Prefrontal cortex and insight in schizophrenia: a volumetric MRI study. Schizophr Res. 2007;89:22-34.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 91]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 92]  [Article Influence: 5.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
27.  Shad MU, Muddasani S, Prasad K, Sweeney JA, Keshavan MS. Insight and prefrontal cortex in first-episode Schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1315-1320.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 93]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 102]  [Article Influence: 5.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
28.  Shad MU, Muddasani S, Keshavan MS. Prefrontal subregions and dimensions of insight in first-episode schizophrenia--a pilot study. Psychiatry Res. 2006;146:35-42.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 98]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 92]  [Article Influence: 5.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
29.  Palaniyappan L, Mallikarjun P, Joseph V, White TP, Liddle PF. Reality distortion is related to the structure of the salience network in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2011;41:1701-1708.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 70]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 75]  [Article Influence: 5.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
30.  Orfei MD, Piras F, Macci E, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G. The neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive insight in schizophrenia. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013;8:418-423.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
31.  Spalletta G, Piras F, Piras F, Caltagirone C, Orfei MD. The structural neuroanatomy of metacognitive insight in schizophrenia and its psychopathological and neuropsychological correlates. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:4729-4740.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 32]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 34]  [Article Influence: 3.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
32.  David A, van Os J, Jones P, Harvey I, Foerster A, Fahy T. Insight and psychotic illness. Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations. Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167:621-628.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 144]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 146]  [Article Influence: 5.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
33.  Rossell SL, Coakes J, Shapleske J, Woodruff PW, David AS. Insight: its relationship with cognitive function, brain volume and symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2003;33:111-119.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 120]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 112]  [Article Influence: 5.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
34.  McFarland J, Cannon DM, Schmidt H, Ahmed M, Hehir S, Emsell L, Barker G, McCarthy P, Elliott MA, McDonald C. Association of grey matter volume deviation with insight impairment in first-episode affective and non-affective psychosis. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;263:133-141.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 26]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 27]  [Article Influence: 2.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
35.  Bassitt DP, Neto MR, de Castro CC, Busatto GF. Insight and regional brain volumes in schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2007;257:58-62.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 59]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 63]  [Article Influence: 3.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
36.  Shad MU, Tamminga CA, Cullum M, Haas GL, Keshavan MS. Insight and frontal cortical function in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Res. 2006;86:54-70.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 112]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 111]  [Article Influence: 6.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
37.  Aleman A, Agrawal N, Morgan KD, David AS. Insight in psychosis and neuropsychological function: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:204-212.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 242]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 239]  [Article Influence: 13.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
38.  Dazzan P, Lloyd T, Morgan KD, Zanelli J, Morgan C, Orr K, Hutchinson G, Fearon P, Allin M, Rifkin L. Neurological abnormalities and cognitive ability in first-episode psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;193:197-202.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 53]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 51]  [Article Influence: 3.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
39.  Antonova E, Sharma T, Morris R, Kumari V. The relationship between brain structure and neurocognition in schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophr Res. 2004;70:117-145.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 291]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 281]  [Article Influence: 14.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
40.  First M, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, William J, Benjamin L.  Users guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM IV Axis II Personality Disorders. Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research: New York 1997; .  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
41.  Birchwood M, Smith J, Drury V, Healy J, Macmillan F, Slade M. A self-report Insight Scale for psychosis: reliability, validity and sensitivity to change. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1994;89:62-67.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 473]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 452]  [Article Influence: 15.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
42.  David A, Kemp R. Five perspectives on the phenomenon of insight in psychosis. Psychiatr Ann. 1997;27:791-797.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 12]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 12]  [Article Influence: 0.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
43.  Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261-276.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
44.  Nelson H, Willison J.  National Adult Reading Test. Windsor: Manual Nfer-Nelson 1991; .  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
45.  Simmons A, Arridge SR, Barker GJ, Williams SC. Simulation of MRI cluster plots and application to neurological segmentation. Magn Reson Imaging. 1996;14:73-92.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 51]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 51]  [Article Influence: 1.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
46.  Reichenberg A, Harvey PD. Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: Integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychol Bull. 2007;133:833-858.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 361]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 364]  [Article Influence: 21.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
47.  Flashman LA, McAllister TW, Johnson SC, Rick JH, Green RL, Saykin AJ. Specific frontal lobe subregions correlated with unawareness of illness in schizophrenia: a preliminary study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2001;13:255-257.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 64]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 75]  [Article Influence: 3.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
48.  Bergé D, Carmona S, Rovira M, Bulbena A, Salgado P, Vilarroya O. Gray matter volume deficits and correlation with insight and negative symptoms in first-psychotic-episode subjects. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2011;123:431-439.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 66]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 79]  [Article Influence: 6.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
49.  Sapara A, Ffytche DH, Birchwood M, Cooke MA, Fannon D, Williams SC, Kuipers E, Kumari V. Preservation and compensation: the functional neuroanatomy of insight and working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2014;152:201-209.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 18]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 18]  [Article Influence: 1.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
50.  van der Meer L, de Vos AE, Stiekema AP, Pijnenborg GH, van Tol MJ, Nolen WA, David AS, Aleman A. Insight in schizophrenia: involvement of self-reflection networks? Schizophr Bull. 2013;39:1288-1295.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 110]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 117]  [Article Influence: 10.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
51.  Sapara A, Ffytche DH, Cooke MA, Williams SC, Kumari V. Is it me? Verbal self-monitoring neural network and clinical insight in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2015;234:328-335.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 16]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 15]  [Article Influence: 1.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
52.  Shad MU, Keshavan MS. Neurobiology of insight deficits in schizophrenia: An fMRI study. Schizophr Res. 2015;165:220-226.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 35]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 35]  [Article Influence: 3.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
53.  Vogeley K, Fink GR. Neural correlates of the first-person-perspective. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:38-42.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 434]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 395]  [Article Influence: 18.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
54.  Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Watkins GL, Ponto LL, Hichwa RD. Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152:1576-1585.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 396]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 377]  [Article Influence: 13.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
55.  Northoff G, Heinzel A, de Greck M, Bermpohl F, Dobrowolny H, Panksepp J. Self-referential processing in our brain--a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage. 2006;31:440-457.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1781]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 1760]  [Article Influence: 97.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
56.  Shenton ME, Dickey CC, Frumin M, McCarley RW. A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2001;49:1-52.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1677]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 1578]  [Article Influence: 68.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
57.  Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Ponto LL, Watkins GL, Hichwa RD. Schizophrenia and cognitive dysmetria: a positron-emission tomography study of dysfunctional prefrontal-thalamic-cerebellar circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:9985-9990.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 438]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 427]  [Article Influence: 15.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
58.  Schmahmann JD. From movement to thought: anatomic substrates of the cerebellar contribution to cognitive processing. Hum Brain Mapp. 1996;4:174-198.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
59.  Picard H, Amado I, Mouchet-Mages S, Olié JP, Krebs MO. The role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia: an update of clinical, cognitive, and functional evidences. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:155-172.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 214]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 202]  [Article Influence: 12.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
60.  Raveendran V, Kumari V. Clinical, cognitive and neural correlates of self-monitoring deficits in schizophrenia: an update. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2007;19:27-37.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 9]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 10]  [Article Influence: 0.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
61.  Kumari V, Fannon D, Ffytche DH, Raveendran V, Antonova E, Premkumar P, Cooke MA, Anilkumar AP, Williams SC, Andrew C. Functional MRI of verbal self-monitoring in schizophrenia: performance and illness-specific effects. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36:740-755.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 47]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 52]  [Article Influence: 3.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
62.  Arnt J, Skarsfeldt T. Do novel antipsychotics have similar pharmacological characteristics? A review of the evidence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;18:63-101.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 537]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 547]  [Article Influence: 21.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
63.  Miyamoto S, Merrill DB, Lieberman JA, Fleischhacker WW, Marder SR. Antipsychotic drugs, in Psychiatry. Tasman A, Kay J, Lieberman JA, First MB, Mario M, editors. USA: John Wiley & Sons, Chichester 2008; 2161-2201.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
64.  Haijma SV, Van Haren N, Cahn W, Koolschijn PC, Hulshoff Pol HE, Kahn RS. Brain volumes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis in over 18 000 subjects. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39:1129-1138.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 627]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 661]  [Article Influence: 60.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]