Sapara A, Ffytche DH, Cooke MA, Williams SC, Kumari V. Voxel-based magnetic resonance imaging investigation of poor and preserved clinical insight in people with schizophrenia. World J Psychiatr 2016; 6(3): 311-321 [PMID: 27679770 DOI: 10.5498/wjp.v6.i3.311]
Corresponding Author of This Article
Veena Kumari, PhD, Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom. veena.kumari@kcl.ac.uk
Research Domain of This Article
Psychiatry
Article-Type of This Article
Case Control Study
Open-Access Policy of This Article
This article is an open-access article which was selected by an in-house editor and fully peer-reviewed by external reviewers. It is distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/
Adegboyega Sapara, Michael A Cooke, Veena Kumari, Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
Dominic H Ffytche, Department of Old Age Psychiatry and Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
Steven CR Williams, Department of Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
Veena Kumari, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
ORCID number: $[AuthorORCIDs]
Author contributions: Sapara A, Ffytche DH and Kumari V designed the study; Cooke MA carried out the neuropsychological assessments; Williams SCR assisted with neuroimaging data acquisition; Sapara A performed all analyses and wrote the manuscript under Ffytche DH and Kumari V’s joint supervision.
Supported by The Wellcome Trust, United Kingdom and was carried out as part of the first author’s PhD research under Professor Veena Kumari and Dr Dominic ffytche’s supervision, Nos. 067427 and 072298; Professor Kumari is part funded by the Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health at the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience King’s College London, and the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, United Kingdom (to Kumari V).
Institutional review board statement: The study procedures had the approval (reference number 209/02) of the ethics committee of the Institute of Psychiatry and South London and Maudsley Foundation NHS Trust, London.
Informed consent statement: All participants provided written informed consent.
Conflict-of-interest statement: None of the authors declare any conflict of interest in this study.
Data sharing statement: The anonymised dataset is available from the first author and the corresponding author.
Open-Access: This article is an open-access article which was selected by an in-house editor and fully peer-reviewed by external reviewers. It is distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/
Correspondence to: Veena Kumari, PhD, Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom. veena.kumari@kcl.ac.uk
Telephone: +44-207-8480233
Received: March 2, 2016 Peer-review started: March 2, 2016 First decision: April 15, 2016 Revised: June 23, 2016 Accepted: July 11, 2016 Article in press: July 13, 2016 Published online: September 22, 2016 Processing time: 90 Days and 12.2 Hours
Abstract
AIM
To define regional grey-matter abnormalities in schizophrenia patients with poor insight (Insight-), relative to patients with preserved clinical insight (Insight+), and healthy controls.
METHODS
Forty stable schizophrenia outpatients (20 Insight- and 20 Insight+) and 20 healthy controls underwent whole brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Insight in all patients was assessed using the Birchwood Insight Scale (BIS; a self-report measure). The two patient groups were pre-selected to match on most clinical and demographic parameters but, by design, they had markedly distinct BIS scores. Voxel-based morphometry employed in SPM8 was used to examine group differences in grey matter volumes across the whole brain.
RESULTS
The three participant groups were comparable in age [F(2,57) = 0.34, P = 0.71] and the patient groups did not differ in age at illness onset [t(38) = 0.87, P = 0.39]. Insight- and Insight+ patient groups also did not differ in symptoms on the Positive and Negative Syndromes scale (PANSS): Positive symptoms [t(38) = 0.58, P = 0.57], negative symptoms [t(38) = 0.61, P = 0.55], general psychopathology [t(38) = 1.30, P = 0.20] and total PANSS scores [t(38) = 0.21, P = 0.84]. The two patient groups, as expected, varied significantly in the level of BIS-assessed insight [t(38) = 12.11, P < 0.001]. MRI results revealed lower fronto-temporal, parahippocampal, occipital and cerebellar grey matter volumes in Insight- patients, relative to Insight+ patients and healthy controls (for all clusters, family-wise error corrected P < 0.05). Insight+ patient and healthy controls did not differ significantly (P > 0.20) from each other.
CONCLUSION
Our findings demonstrate a clear association between poor clinical insight and smaller fronto-temporal, occipital and cerebellar grey matter volumes in stable long-term schizophrenia patients.
Core tip: Poor clinical insight is the most prevalent symptom in patients with schizophrenia and is of growing importance due to its direct association with poor clinical outcomes, such as frequent relapses and hospital admissions. This study identified significantly reduced fronto-temporal, parahippocampal, occipital and cerebellar grey matter volumes in Insight- patients relative to both Insight+ patients and healthy controls. The involvement of multiple brain areas and corresponding neural networks supports the theory that clinical insight, as a neurological function, is not confined to specific neuroanatomical regions but probably a function of a complex neurocognitive interplay with contributions from multiple neural networks.
Citation: Sapara A, Ffytche DH, Cooke MA, Williams SC, Kumari V. Voxel-based magnetic resonance imaging investigation of poor and preserved clinical insight in people with schizophrenia. World J Psychiatr 2016; 6(3): 311-321
Nearly a century ago, Kraepelin (1919) observed that schizophrenia patients often had “no real understanding of the gravity of their disorder” and regularly disputed that they suffer from a mental illness[1]. In the 1930s, Lewis described clinical insight as having “a correct attitude to a morbid change in one’s self”[2,3] and low clinical insight is the most prevalent symptom occurring in about 97% of schizophrenia patients[2,4]. Impaired insight in schizophrenia is of growing importance due to its direct correlation with poor clinical outcomes, such as frequent relapses and hospital admissions[5], poor compliance with medication and treatment plans[6-8], severe psychopathology[9], greater suicidal tendencies and self-injurious behaviour[9-12]. Some studies reporting positive correlations between improvement in clinical insight and better global clinical impression and clinical outcome scores[13] have further suggested the adoption of clinical insight as a possible therapeutic target in schizophrenia patients[14].
Similarities between impaired insight in schizophrenia and unawareness of neurological deficits such as anosognosia, first described in patients with acute brain lesions with left-sided hemiplegia who were unaware of the impairments in their paralysed limbs[15,16], led to the notion that both phenomena share a common neurological basis[17-19] and prompted investigations of neuroanatomical abnormalities in relation to impaired clinical insight in schizophrenia. Earliest studies, using computerized tomography (CT) scan, reported significant and direct associations between impaired clinical insight and ventricular enlargement[20], total insight scores and total brain volumes[21] and a linear relationship between global cortical atrophy and impaired clinical insight[22]. These studies all concluded that there is a significant association between whole brain volume loss and impaired clinical insight in schizophrenia. Structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies also reported correlations between impaired clinical insight and smaller regional grey matter volumes, including the frontal lobe, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), posterior cingulate, temporal and parietal lobes[23-28]. More recently, correlations have been reported between impaired insight and smaller right posterior insula volumes[29], smaller grey matter volumes of the right ventro-lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC)[30], left ventrolateral PFC, right dorsolateral PFC, insula, bilateral premotor area and the putamen; and reduced white matter volumes of the right superior longitudinal fasciculum, left corona radiata, left forceps minor and bilateral cingulum[31].
Although most studies have reported a correlation between brain volume loss and impaired insight, some studies failed to find any correlation between clinical insight and either ventricular or total/regional brain volumes[3,32,33], while others reported associations between impaired clinical insight and increased (rather than decreased) right medial orbitofrontal cortex grey matter volumes[28], and between symptom misattribution and increased grey matter volumes in bilateral caudate regions, right thalamus, left insula, putamen and cerebellum[34]. Bassitt et al[35] found no significant inverse correlation between total or regional grey matter volumes and clinical insight but, contrary to their expectations, observed a positive correlation between degree of insight impairment and the left medial PFC and ACC grey matter volumes, which they attributed to higher doses of antipsychotics given to patients with impaired clinical insight in their sample. The marked variation in findings may be due to the use of different brain volumetric assessment techniques, the heterogeneity of clinical insight measures and varying clinical characteristics of schizophrenia patients studied[25,35,36].
The aim of the present study was to characterise grey matter alterations in stable long-term schizophrenia outpatients with impaired clinical insight by directly comparing them, for the first time to our knowledge, with schizophrenia outpatients with preserved clinical insight, matched on average for age, sex and relevant demographic and clinical characteristics. Our approach of utilising the two extremes of the insight distribution should yield the largest structural difference in relation to insight. We also compared how these distinct groups of patients might differ from healthy controls, matched on average on age and sex of the patient groups. Based on the findings (where positive) of existing studies involving solely or predominantly chronic patient samples, we hypothesised that, patients with impaired insight (Insight-) will show smaller frontal and temporal regional grey matter volumes compared to patients with preserved insight (Insight+) and healthy controls. This hypothesis also has support from previous studies showing, on average, poor cognitive function in patients with impaired insight[25,37,38] and a positive association between grey matter volumes of these regions and a range of cognitive functions in schizophrenia[39].
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants and study design
This study included 60 right-handed participants. Forty of these were patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, confirmed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID)[40]. The patients formed two groups of 20 patients each, pre-selected to have preserved and impaired insight, out of a larger pool of 70 stable community patients. The assessment of insight and differentiating criteria are described in detail under “clinical assessment”. All included patients were required to be: (1) on well established antipsychotic medication doses for ≥ 3 mo; (2) in the stable (chronic) phase of the illness; and (3) ≥ 2 years from illness onset. Twenty healthy controls screened to exclude neuropsychiatric conditions and matched, on average, for age and sex of the patients were studied for comparison purposes. Ethics approval was granted by the ethics committee of the Institute of Psychiatry and South London and Maudsley Foundation NHS Trust, London. All participants provided written informed consent.
Clinical assessment
Birchwood Insight Scale (BIS)[41], a self-rated questionnaire, was used to assess insight in all patients. The BIS measures three different aspects of clinical insight[2]: (1) the awareness of the presence of a mental disorder (2nd and 7th statement); (2) the awareness of the need for treatment (3rd, 6th statement); and (3) the ability to label symptoms as abnormal (1st and 8th statement). Each individual BIS statement (8 in total) is rated and given a score of one for unsure, and either 0 or 2 for agree and disagree, depending on whether agreeing with the statement depicts preserved clinical insight (all statements are corrected for response valence). As we did not include any inpatients, Item 4 “My stay in hospital is necessary” was deleted, thus yielding a maximum possible score of 14, compared with a maximum possible score of 16 in the full scale BIS. In operationalising the BIS, Birchwood et al[41] classified preserved insight as having a minimum score of 9 (out of 14). In this study, we defined “preserved insight” as a minimum score of 13 (out of 14) and “impaired insight” as a score of 8 or below. This rather conservative method was designed to ensure that the two groups had distinct levels of insight and also to eliminate those with partial clinical insight levels. All patients were supervised during the completion of the BIS. The BIS has acceptable internal consistency (α = 0.75) and one week test-retest reliability (r = 0.90 for the total score[41]), and insight assessed on the BIS correlates positively with scores on other measures of insight[10,26,42]. For sample characterization purposes, symptoms in patients were assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scales (PANSS[43]). In addition, predicted IQ of all study participants was measured using the National Adult Reading Test (NART[44]).
Image acquisition and processing
Whole brain MRI scans were acquired for all study participants using a 1.5 Tesla GE NV/I Signa system (General Electric, Milwaukee WI, United States) at the Maudsley Hospital, London. A series of sagittal fast gradient echo scout images were obtained to correct for head tilt and to orient subsequent images relative to the anterior-commissure/posterior-commissure line and the interhemispheric fissure. A 3-D inversion recovery prepared fast spoiled GRASS sequence was applied to acquire T1-weighted images in the axial plane with 1.5 mm contiguous sections (TR = 18 ms, T1 = 450 ms, TE = 5.1 ms, flip angle = 20o with one data average and a 256 × 256 × 128 voxel matrix). Acquisition parameters were selected employing a sophisticated image simulation[45]. All MRI images were converted into ANALYZE format (ANALYZE software, BRU, Mayo Foundation, Rochester, MN) and pre-processed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8; http://wwwfil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) running in MATLAB 2006a (MathWorks, Natick, MA). Customised T1-weighted templates of the whole brain, grey matter, white matter and cerebro-spinal fluid (CSF) were created for patient and healthy participant groups separately, and also for the whole study sample combined.
Statistical analysis
Demographic and clinical measures: Possible group differences in age, education and NART IQ were examined using analyses of variance (ANOVAs), and significant Group effects were followed by independent sample t-tests. Possible differences between the two patient groups in clinical variables (age at illness onset, PANSS symptom scores and medication) were examined using independent sample t-tests. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 22, with alpha level for significance testing maintained at P≤ 0.05 (two-tailed), unless stated otherwise.
MRI: Group differences (healthy controls vs Insight- patients, health controls vs Insight+ paitents, and Insight+vs Insight- patients) in grey matter volumes, across the whole brain, were examined using ANOVA in SPM8 (height threshold P < 0.005; familywise-error (FWE)-corrected at the cluster level P < 0.05). To rule out the possibility that any observed group differences were due to trend-level Group differences in education and IQ (see RESULTS, demographic and clinical measures), group differences in grey matter volumes were re-evaluated using analysis of co-variance, with education and IQ entered as co-variates.
RESULTS
Demographic and clinical characteristics
The three participant groups did not differ in age [F(2,57 = 0.34, P = 0.71]. There were trend level effects of Group in years of education [F(2,57) = 2.60, P = 0.08] and NART IQ [F(2,57) = 2.67, P = 0.08]. Healthy controls spent more years in education than Insight- patients [t(38) = 2.11, P = 0.04] but differed only at a trend level when compared with Insight+ patients [t(38) = 1.77, P = 0.08]. Healthy controls also had higher NART IQ than Insight- patients [t(38) = 2.47, P = 0.02] but did not differ from Insight+ patients [t(38) = 1.19, P = 0.24]. There were no significant differences the Insight- and Insight+ patient groups in education [t(38) = 0.06, P = 0.95] and NART IQ [t(38) = 1.04, P = 0.31] (Table 1). The two patient groups were similar in age at illness onset [t(38) = 0.87, P = 0.39], positive symptoms [t(38) = 0.58, P = 0.57], negative symptoms [t(38) = 0.61, P = 0.55], general psychopathology [t(38) = 1.30, P = 0.20] and total PANSS symptoms [t(38) = 0.21, P = 0.84]. Patients in the two groups were on a range of typical and atypical antipsychotics (Table 1) but received, on average, similar doses of antipsychotic medication [t(38) = 0.86, P = 0.40]. The Insight+ patient group, confirming our insight-based pre-selection, had significantly higher BIS score than the Insight- group [t(38) = 12.11, P < 0.001].
Table 1 Demographics and clinical characteristics of the study groups.
Healthy controls (n = 20; 15 male, 5 female)
Patients insight+ group (n = 20; 16 male, 4 female)
Patients insight- group (n = 20; 16 male, 4 female)
2 (1 on clozapine + levomepromazine, 1 zuclopenthixol + aripiprazole)
MRI: Group effects in regional grey matter volumes
Group differences in brain MRI grey matter volumes are presented in Table 2, and described below.
Table 2 Group differences in grey matter volumes (height threshold P < 0.005).
Groups
BA
Size
Side
MNI
T value
Cluster P FWE-corrected unless in italics
Voxel P FWE-corrected
X
Y
Z
Insight+ > Insight- patients
Superior temporal gyrus
22
46555
R
63
-3
5
4.91
0.001
0.020
45
20
-33
4.74
0.034
66
-8
4
4.68
0.040
Precentral gyrus
4
66
-5
22
4.55
0.057
Inferior frontal gyrus
47
54
19
0
4.52
0.063
Precentral gyrus
6
64
0
26
4.40
0.088
Postcentral gyrus
43
66
-8
16
4.33
0.106
parahippocampus
28
14
0
-27
4.07
0.406
Inferior frontal gyrus
47
103898
L
-41
15
-6
4.81
< 0.001
0.027
Middle frontal gyrus
9
-37
19
35
4.74
0.034
Inferior frontal gyrus
47
-37
15
-10
4.73
0.035
-35
20
-10
4.54
0.059
Precentral gyrus
44
-59
8
7
4.39
0.091
Superior temporal gyrus
22
-62
-4
8
4.36
0.097
Precentral gyrus
6
-60
4
6
4.33
0.107
Middle temporal gyrus
21
-35
-3
-23
4.27
0.126
parahippocampal gyrus
20
-34
-5
-28
4.16
0.166
Cuneus
18
35993
L
-5
-83
5
4.43
0.003
0.082
Cerebellum
-
R
35
-90
-17
4.26
0.129
Cuneus
18
R
26
-93
-18
3.90
0.305
Cerebellum
-
R
4
-61
2
3.88
0.317
Cuneus
18
R
5
-98
10
3.50
0.630
R
5
-96
3
3.44
0.674
Cerebellum
-
L
-36
-82
-15
3.38
0.730
Insight- > Insight+ patients
Nil significant
Healthy controls > Insight- patients
Inferior frontal gyrus
47
35300
L
-49
19
-3
4.63
0.004
0.046
Superior temporal gyrus
22
-60
1
3
4.30
0.115
Inferior frontal gyrus
47
-41
18
-5
4.21
0.144
-38
22
-8
4.19
0.153
-36
-1
-14
3.86
0.333
Inferior temporal gyrus
20
-28
-14
-41
3.61
0.530
Parahippocamal gyrus
34
-13
4
-23
3.58
0.552
Middle frontal gyrus
11
-42
40
-19
3.39
0.722
Inferior occipital gyrus
18
11168
L
-38
-92
-2
4.51
0.034
0.065
Middle occipital gyrus
19
-52
-76
-10
4.29
0.117
-48
-80
-14
3.96
0.266
-49
-81
7
3.90
0.302
Middle temporal gyrus
39/
-53
-72
22
3.37
0.740
19
-52
-74
18
3.33
0.768
-49
-76
20
3.29
0.797
Cerebellum
-
25235
R
35
-90
-17
4.46
0.016
0.074
(posterior lobe)
11
-90
-37
4.21
0.146
Occipital lobe
18
23
-94
-18
4.01
0.238
Cerebellum
34
-85
-40
3.93
0.355
(posterior lobe)
38
-82
-41
3.91
0.489
Insight- patients > healthy controls
Nil significant
Healthy control > Insight+ patients
Nil significant
Insight+ patients > healthy controls
Nil significant
Insight-vs Insight+ patients: Compared to Insight- patients, Insight+ patients had larger grey matter volumes in the inferior frontal and precentral gyri, superior and middle temporal gyri, parahippocampus, cuneus and cerebellum of both cerebral hemispheres (Figure 1).
Figure 1 Images showing regions of decreased grey matter volume in the impaired insight patient group, relative to the preserved insight patient and healthy controls (maps thresholded at P = 0.
005; left = right).
Healthy controls vs Insight- patients: Compared to Insight- patients, healthy controls had larger grey matter volumes in the left inferior and middle frontal gyri, left superior, middle and inferior temporal gyri, left parahippocampus, right cerebellum, and bilateral superior, middle and inferior occipital gyri (Figure 1).
Healthy controls vs Insight+ patients: There were no significant differences between healthy controls and Insight+ patients.
Group differences after co-varying for education and predicted IQ
Differences in grey matter volumes (noted earlier) between healthy controls and Insight- patients remained present but with reduced significance when we co-varied for education and IQ (Table 3). Group differences between Insight- and Insight+ patients, however, were not affected.
Table 3 Group differences in grey matter volumes after co-varying for education and National Adult Reading Test IQ (height threshold P < 0.005).
Groups
BA
Size
Side
MNI
T value
Cluster P FWE-corrected unless shown in italics
Voxel P FWE-corrected
X
Y
Z
Insight+ > Insight- patients
Superior Temporal gyrus
22
37261
R
63
-3
5
4.70
0.002
0.044
45
20
-33
4.56
0.066
66
-8
4
4.45
0.088
Precentral gyrus
4
66
-5
22
4.44
0.092
Inferior frontal gyrus
47
54
19
0
4.39
0.103
Precentral gyrus
6
64
0
26
4.28
0.137
Postcentral gyrus
43
66
-8
16
4.15
0.192
Inferior frontal gyrus
47
65047
L
-42
16
-4
4.65
< 0.001
0.050
-38
14
-8
4.65
0.052
-36
18
-10
4.52
0.073
Middle frontal gyrus
9
-37
19
35
4.52
0.073
Superior temporal gyrus
22
-61
-2
7
4.28
0.139
Precentral gyrus
44
-59
9
9
4.17
0.184
Parahippocampual gyrus
21
-34
-3
-36
4.10
0.213
Cuneus
18
24291
L
-5
-83
5
4.32
0.014
0.125
Cerebellum
-
R
35
-90
-17
4.17
0.181
Cuneus
18
R
26
-93
-18
3.73
0.466
Cerebellum
-
R
4
-61
2
3.80
0.409
Cuneus
18
R
5
-98
10
3.35
0.787
Medial frontal gyrus
10
16854
L
0
60
3
3.98
0.050
0.285
Superior frontal gyrus
9
0
51
26
3.64
0.544
Insight- > Insight+ patients
Nil significant
Healthy controls > Insight- patients
Inferior frontal gyrus
47
9770
L
-51
19
-2
3.68
0.036
0.511
Superior temporal gyrus
38
-21
5
-24
3.35
0.786
Inferior frontal gyrus
47
-26
18
-7
3.34
0.796
Parahippocampal gyrus
34
-16
4
-23
3.29
0.827
Inferior occipital gyrus
18
4935
L
-38
-92
-2
3.92
0.122
0.323
Middle occipital gyrus
19
-52
-76
-10
3.70
0.494
-44
-83
8
3.37
0.775
Middle temporal gyrus
18
-43
-81
13
3.22
0.873
Cerebellum (posterior lobe)
-
6085
R
35
-90
-17
3.68
0.089
0.304
11
-90
-37
3.60
0.378
Occipital lobe
18
28
-94
-16
3.32
0.656
23
-94
-18
3.26
0.713
Insight- patients > healthy controls
Nil significant
Healthy controls > Insight+ patients
Nil significant
Insight+ patients > healthy controls
Nil significant
DISCUSSION
In this study, we directly compared two matched groups of schizophrenia patients but with distinct levels of clinical insight (Insight-vs Insight+) and investigated how they differ from each other and also from healthy controls in regional grey matter volumes examined using voxel-based morphometry (VBM) technique. We tested the hypothesis that Insight- patients will show smaller frontal and temporal grey matter volumes compared to Insight+ patients. All three participant groups were comparable in age and the two patient groups were similar in all demographic and clinical parameters, including age at illness onset, years of education, NART IQ, symptoms (PANSS scores) and doses of medication prescribed. Insight- patients, however, had lower IQ and fewer years in education than healthy controls. Although, on average, lower IQ as well as deficits in many specific cognitive domains in patients with schizophrenia, relative to the healthy population, are commonly reported[46], our study suggests that this may be particularly true for those with impaired insight[37] and in turn may also explain the finding of significantly fewer years in education in the Insight- (but not Insight+) patient group, compared with the healthy controls. The patient groups scored at opposing ends of the BIS scale; this allows for the interpretation of observed neuroanatomical differences in relation to clinical insight levels of the respective patient group.
As hypothesized, we found that Insight- patients had smaller grey matter volumes than Insight+ patients, bilaterally in the frontal and temporal lobes (mainly in the inferior frontal and precentral gyri and superior and middle temporal gyri), as well as in the parahippocamal gyrus, occipital lobes (including the cuneus) and the cerebellum. Insight- patients also showed similar grey matter deficits, particularly on the left, when compared to healthy controls (Figure 1).
Our findings of smaller fronto-temporal regional grey matter volumes are in accordance with previous imaging studies, that used the “Region of Interest” (ROI) approach and found a significant and direct correlation between smaller frontal areas, including the dorsolateral PFC, inferior frontal and middle frontal gyri[22,26-28,47,48] and impaired clinical insight. Early reports of poor executive functioning in schizophrenia patients with impaired insight, similar to those with frontal lobe lesions, initiated the interest in the integrity of the frontal lobe in schizophrenia. Since then, several studies[26,30,31,47], including this one, have reported frontal neuroanatomical abnormalities in relation to impaired clinical insight in schizophrenia. Some functional imaging studies have further associated aberrant frontal functional MRI activity with impaired clinical insight during working memory[49], self-reflection[50], self-monitoring[51] and self-awareness tasks[52] in schizophrenia. In addition, earlier correlational VBM studies have also reported associations between smaller superior and middle temporal lobe grey matter volumes and impaired clinical insight[23,48].
Our other finding of smaller cuneus and occipital grey matter volumes in Insight- patients is also broadly in agreement with the earlier reported association between poor symptom relabelling dimension of clinical insight and smaller grey matter volumes of the precuneus, cuneus and medial occipital gyrus by Morgan et al[25]. Unlike Morgan et al[25], we did not investigate preferential or predominant contribution of particular insight dimensions because the BIS subscale scores in our sample were highly positively correlated with each other (rho = 0.50-0.882; P < 0.001). This might be due to our sampling methods that ensured that our Insight- and Insight+ patient groups had markedly different insight levels, possibly in all domains. Other VBM studies have also reported an association between the smaller precuneus grey matter volumes and lower insight in schizophrenia[23]. The role of the precuneus has been described in the facilitation of increased awareness into one’s mental states[23,53] and has also been implicated, in conjunction with other midline structures, in the self-appraisal processes[54,55]. Compared to anterior cortical regions, much less is known about the involvement of posterior medial cortices due to the dearth of research into the contributions of these brain regions to various aspects of psychotic disorders[25]. In our recent study, we found further evidence of functional contributions from the precuneus, as well as the cerebellum, in supporting neural activities sub-serving the preservation of insight in schizophrenia patients[49].
There have been previous reports of cerebellar atrophy, on average, in schizophrenia patients[56]. A previous study[48] also observed a significant association between impaired clinical insight and reduced bilateral cerebellar grey matter volumes in schizophrenia, and that this relationship was not associated with any specific dimension of clinical insight. Other studies have described the involvement of the cerebellum in higher cognitive functioning, with its extensive connectivity with limbic structures, including the parahippocampal gyrus, and associated cortical areas involved in cognition and executive function[57,58], and this has been implicated in the neuropathology of schizophrenia and poor clinical insight[48,59]. Our recent finding of increased cerebellar activity, detected using fMRI, in Insight+ patients compared to Insight- patients, during a working memory task, also indicated cerebellar involvement in the preservation of clinical insight in schizophrenia[49].
In accordance with the observations made by other studies, we also found grey matter reductions in many areas in Insight- patients, compared to healthy controls[48]. These differences remained, but became less significant, after we co-varied for education and NART IQ. Co-varying for education and NART IQ had no effects on grey matter volume differences between preserved and Insight- patient groups, most likely because these two groups were comparable on these parameters.
Strengths and limitations
We employed a direct comparison method between distinct groups of schizophrenia patients (Insight- and Insight+) with closely matched demographic and clinical qualities, thereby facilitating valid comparisons and inferences. The study also had 60 participants (n = 20 per group) and thus was adequately powered for the observations made. We were, however, limited in our ability to explore the effects of sex on brain volumes and in the observed group differences, as our sample was predominantly male. Nonetheless, male:female ratios were similar and any possible effect is expected to be uniform in all groups. Also, although the patient groups were comparable in all relevant areas, our healthy controls had more education than our patient groups, and had higher IQ scores than Insight- patient group, although co-varying for these differences did not change the pattern of observed group differences. By adopting a direct comparison method between matched patient groups at the extremes of insight measures, we minimised confounding effects of partial insight levels and were able to exclude overall effects of schizophrenia on brain volumes. However, in as much as we endeavoured that our two patient groups are highly comparable but for their insight levels, there are possibilities of other differential properties, such as brain functional properties, which could possibly contribute to our findings. Lastly, patients in both the Insight+ and Insight- groups were on a range of atypical and typical antipsychotics (Table 1) which vary in their pharmacological profiles[60,61] as well as in their effects on brain volumes[62]. This may have influenced the results we observed in this study.
In conclusion, schizophrenia patients with impaired insight patients have smaller fronto-temporal, parahippocampal, occipital and cerebellar grey matter volumes, compared with preserved insight schizophrenia patients and healthy controls. The involvement of multiple brain areas and corresponding neural networks supports the theory that clinical insight, as a neurological function, is not confined to specific neuroanatomical regions in the brain but probably a function of a complex neurocognitive interplay with contributions from neural networks, including working memory and executive functioning, self-monitoring and awareness and others[19,23,49,63,64].
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We are grateful to the MRI unit, Maudsley Hospital for their help with data acquisition.
COMMENTS
Background
Impaired insight in schizophrenia is found to have a direct correlation with poor clinical outcomes, such as frequent relapses and hospital admissions, poor compliance with medication, greater suicidal tendencies and self-injurious behaviour. Some studies reporting positive correlations between improvement in clinical insight and better clinical outcomes have further suggested the adoption of clinical insight as a possible therapeutic target in schizophrenia patients.
Research frontiers
The ability to target insight therapeutically is highly complex and remains elusive to most methods trialled so far. The identification of the underpinning neural correlates of clinical insight will aid the development of specific treatment strategies aimed at improving insight in schizophrenia.
Innovations and breakthroughs
The study reported in this manuscript is distinct from all previous studies in this area (mostly correlational) in that it identifies regional grey matter abnormalities in stable schizophrenia outpatients with impaired clinical insight, relative to those with preserved clinical insight (impaired and preserved insight groups scoring at extreme ends of a multidimensional insight scale but matched on age, sex and other symptoms) as well healthy controls, using a categorical approach. The authors found a clear association between impaired clinical insight and smaller fronto-temporal, occipital and cerebellar grey matter volumes in stable long-term schizophrenia patients.
Applications
Clinical insight, as a neurological function, is likely to be dependent on complex neurocognitive interplay with contributions from multiple neural networks.
Terminology
Voxel-based-morphometry is a neuroimaging analysis technique in which structural brain properties are examined on a voxel-by-voxel basis and reported in standardized coordinates. Clinical insight refers to a patient’s complex state of awareness of his or her own mental disorder.
Peer-review
The study is well designed and the manuscript is clearly written and easy to read all throughout.
Footnotes
Manuscript source: Invited manuscript
Specialty type: Psychiatry
Country of origin: United Kingdom
Peer-review report classification
Grade A (Excellent): A
Grade B (Very good): B
Grade C (Good): C
Grade D (Fair): 0
Grade E (Poor): 0
P- Reviewer: Belli H, Eduardo JA, Tampi RR S- Editor: Ji FF L- Editor: A E- Editor: Lu YJ
McEvoy JP, Freter S, Everett G, Geller JL, Appelbaum P, Apperson LJ, Roth L. Insight and the clinical outcome of schizophrenic patients.J Nerv Ment Dis. 1989;177:48-51.
[PubMed] [DOI][Cited in This Article: ]
Takai A, Uematsu M, Ueki H, Sone K. Insight and its related factors in chronic schizophrenic patients: A preliminary study.Eur J Psychiatry. 1992;6:159-170.
[PubMed] [DOI][Cited in This Article: ]
Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia.Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261-276.
[PubMed] [DOI][Cited in This Article: ]